
Publication details
Publisher: Springer
Place: Berlin
Year: 1992
Pages: 67-82
Series: Recent Research in Psychology
ISBN (Hardback): 9780387977003
Full citation:
, "Operationism, scientism, and the rhetoric of power", in: Positivism in psychology, Berlin, Springer, 1992


Operationism, scientism, and the rhetoric of power
pp. 67-82
in: Charles W. Tolman (ed), Positivism in psychology, Berlin, Springer, 1992Abstract
Operational definitions are definitely alive and well within psychology. For example, Underwood wrote, "Most experimentalists in all disciplines tacitly or actively accept operational definitions as a means of specifying the empirical basis of a discipline. That is, they use operational definitions in order to define the phenomena of nature with which a discipline concerns itself (1966, p. 299). Several other authors of texts on experimental design echo Underwood's sentiments (e.g., Bachrach, 1965; Christensen, 1980; Kidder & Charles, 1986; Lyons, 1965; McGuigan, 1983; Myers, 1980; Neale & Liebert, 1973; Solso & Johnson, 1984).
Publication details
Publisher: Springer
Place: Berlin
Year: 1992
Pages: 67-82
Series: Recent Research in Psychology
ISBN (Hardback): 9780387977003
Full citation:
, "Operationism, scientism, and the rhetoric of power", in: Positivism in psychology, Berlin, Springer, 1992